Neurogenesis interferes with past learning in infant and adult mice
May 12, 2014
By Helen Shen and Nature magazine
“The finding was very surprising to us initially. Most people think
new neurons mean better memory,” says Sheena Josselyn, a neuroscientist
who led the study together with her husband Paul Frankland at the
Hospital for Sick Children in Toronto, Canada.
Humans, mice and several other mammals grow new neurons in the
hippocampus throughout their lives — rapidly at first, but more and more
slowly with age. Researchers have previously shown that boosting neural
proliferation before learning can enhance memory formation in adult
mice. But the latest study shows that after information is learned,
neuron growth can degrade those memories.
Although seemingly counterintuitive, the disruptive role of these
neurons makes some sense, says Josselyn. She notes that some theoretical
models have predicted such an effect. “More neurons increase the
capacity to learn new memories in the future,” she says. “But memory is
based on a circuit, so if you add to this circuit, it makes sense that
it would disrupt it.” Newly added neurons could have a useful role in
clearing old memories and making way for new ones, says Josselyn.
Forgetting curve
The researchers tested newborn and adult mice on a conditioning task,
training the animals to fear an environment in which they received
repeated electric shocks. All the mice learned the task quickly, but
whereas infant mice remembered the negative experience for only one day
after training, adult mice retained the negative memory for several
weeks.
This difference seems to correlate with differences in neural
proliferation. Josselyn and her team were able to enhance memory
persistence in newborn mice by genetically and chemically suppressing
growth of new neurons after learning. And in adult mice, four to six
weeks of regular exercise — an activity known to promote neuron
proliferation — reduced the persistence of previously learned fear.
The genetic and chemical manipulations cannot be applied readily to
humans, so the findings will be difficult to pursue in people, says
Josselyn. But both mice and humans have ‘infantile amnesia’, or
pronounced forgetting of early life experiences. Josselyn says that
rapid neurogenesis in many young animals could help to explain the
phenomenon across species.
The researchers also examined learned fear in guinea pigs and in
Chilean rodents called degus — both of which have longer gestation
periods than mice, and correspondingly reduced brain growth after birth.
Baby degus and guinea pigs do not have infantile amnesia, but the
researchers were able to mimic its effects in the animals through
exercise or drugs that promote neuron growth.
“It's incredibly impressive. They covered everything from genetic and
pharmacological interventions, to behavioral interventions, to
cross-species comparisons," says Karl Deisseroth, a neuroscientist at
Stanford University in California who is collaborating with the group on
a separate project but did not contribute to the current study.
Deisseroth, who in 2005 published the computational model Josselyn
alludes to, says he is excited to see strong experimental validation of
the idea nearly ten years later.
Amar Sahay, a neuroscientist at Massachusetts General Hospital in
Boston, says that most previous work, including his own, has focused on
the effects of neurogenesis before memory formation. The latest work, by
examining what happens after learning, paints a more complete picture
of the relationship between neurogenesis and long-term memory
management. “It’s a very exciting study,” says Sahay.
Article retrieved from: http://www.scientificamerican.com/article/new-brain-cells-erase-old-memories/?&WT.mc_id=SA_DD_20140512
Image retrieved from: http://www.scientificamerican.com/sciam/cache/file/27A9CD14-CCF7-4871-92FBCF826651334F_article.jpg?FF40E
0 comments:
Post a Comment